Childhood Caffeine Exposure May Negatively Affect Cognitive Functioning

This is Archived content. This content is available for historical purposes only. It may not reflect the current state of science or language from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA). For current information, please visit nida.nih.gov.

This study found that:

Image
Close-up of child’s hands playing with a Rubik’s cube
Image Courtesy of pexels.com, CC0
  • In children ages 9 to 10 years, greater caffeine intake was associated with reductions in several measures of cognitive functioning.
  • For most assessed cognitive functions, this negative correlation remained after controlling for variables such as age, gender, socioeconomic status, and sleep.

Many adolescents and young children regularly drink beverages that contain caffeine. In the United States, children ages 2 to 12 years consume an average of 37 mg of caffeine per day. In adults, consumption of less than 400 mg of caffeine per day—roughly the amount found in 4 cups of coffee, 10 cans of cola, or 2 energy shot drinks—is not thought to present any health risks and may provide benefits such as improved concentration, alertness, or even athletic performance. Higher doses, however, can adversely affect mood, sleep, and physical and cognitive performance. Children have lower tolerance for caffeine than adults, making their access to caffeinated beverages such as cola and energy drinks particularly concerning. A new study supports these concerns, demonstrating that caffeine consumption during childhood can impede children’s cognitive function.

Caffeine Intake Is Associated With Reduced Cognitive Functions

Dr. Anqi Qiu and colleagues from the Department of Biomedical Engineering at the National University of Singapore investigated the effects of caffeine intake on cognitive functioning in almost 12,000 children ages 9 to 10 years who participate in the NIDA-funded Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. This longitudinal cohort study examines potential relationships between substance use and neurocognition in U.S. children and adolescents. In their analysis, Dr. Qiu and colleagues included children for whom data on caffeine consumption were available, as well as results on seven tests assessing:

Image
Figure. Caffeine Intake in Children Is Associated With Sociodemographic Variables and Reduced Cognitive Functioning  Colors indicate Spearman’s correlation values (ρ), with shades of red signifying positive correlations and shades of blue signifying negative correlations. Greater caffeine intake was associated with lower socioeconomic status, more sleep disturbances, older age, and male gender. Additionally, greater caffeine consumption was negatively correlated with all seven cognitive functions assessed. See full text description at end of article.
  • Vocabulary comprehension.
  • Reading decoding.
  • Inhibitory control.
  • Working memory.
  • Episodic memory.
  • Cognitive flexibility.
  • Processing speed.

About two-thirds of the children reported any consumption of caffeinated drinks (coffee, espresso, tea, soda, or energy drinks), with an average intake of 13.0±43.7 mg caffeine per day; almost 10 percent consumed three or more types of such beverages. The analyses demonstrated that greater caffeine consumption was associated with worse performance on all seven cognitive measures assessed (see Figure). Children with a lower socioeconomic status showed higher caffeine consumption and worse cognitive performance on all seven measures. Similarly, an association existed between more sleep disturbances, greater caffeine intake, and worse performance on four of the seven measures. Age and gender also influenced caffeine consumption and cognitive performance. However, even after adjusting for age, gender, socioeconomic status, and sleep, increased caffeine intake was still associated with reduced vocabulary comprehension, cognitive flexibility, processing speed, working memory, and episodic memory.

Direction of causation cannot be determined from a simple association, and it is conceivable that children who already have greater cognitive challenges consume more caffeine, but the results raise the possibility that caffeine intake may negatively influence cognitive development in children. This is a significant public health concern because of the large number of children who regularly consume caffeinated drinks. “As beverages with caffeine are consumed frequently, controlling their intake may reduce a risk for nonoptimal cognitive development in children,” says Dr. Qiu, the study’s senior author. She adds, “Thus, from a public health perspective, we advise parents to control their children’s intake of beverages with caffeine as a precaution to reduce the risk of interference with normal cognitive development.” She also notes that further study is required to explore the complex pharmacologic and neural mechanisms of caffeine in the body to better understand its effects on each specific cognitive function.

The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study was supported by NIDA grants DA041022, DA041028, DA041048, DA041089, DA041106, DA041117, DA041120, DA041134, DA041148, DA041156, DA041174, DA041123, and DA041147.

Text Description of Figure

The figure illustrates the correlation between caffeine intake in childhood and sociodemographic variables as well as cognitive functioning. Terms on the left indicate the main categories of variables, and terms in the middle indicate the specific variables assessed. Colored rectangles indicate the strength of the correlations, and a scale at the right identifies the correlation coefficients associated with each color. The scale ranges from dark blue at the bottom, representing a correlation value of -1.0, to increasingly lighter shades of blue to white, representing a correlation value of 0, to increasingly darker shades of red, and finally dark red representing a correlation value of +1.0.

There were four sociodemographic factors and sleep, indicated in gray. For socioeconomic status (row 1), the correlation value with caffeine intake was about -0.5 (medium blue), for sleep (row 2), it was about +0.1 (very light pink), for age (row 3) it was about +0.3 (darker pink), and for gender (row 4) it was about -0.2 (light blue).

There were two crystallized cognitive skills, indicated in green. For vocabulary comprehension (row 5) the correlation value was about -0.4 (medium blue) and for reading (row 6) it was about -0.2 (light blue).

There were five fluid cognitive skills, indicated in purple. For inhibition control (row 7), the correlation value was about -0.1 (very light blue), and, for working memory (row 8), cognitive flexibility (row 9), processing speed (row 10), and episodic memory (row 11) it was about -0.2 (light blue).

Source: